Think you’re bad at math? There’s a reason for that. (2024)

Americans have a love-hate relationship with mathematics. On the one hand, we understand that success in our technology-dependent world requires proficiency in mathematics, and if we don’t cultivate this proficiency in students, we may languish behind those who do. On the other hand, we’re just bad at it.

Research seems to support this view. The National Assessment of Educational Progress found that, in 2015, just 25 percent of 12th graders performed at or above proficiency in mathematics. Nor are we doing well when compared to other countries. The United States’ mathematics performance score (474 mean score) falls below the average for all OECD countries (494). Meanwhile, Japan, China, and Singapore are crushing it (mean scores 539, 540, and 564 respectively).

Is it any wonder that the refrain “I’m not a math person” has become hackneyed? This defense contains a troubling subtext: Some people are born good at math, some aren’t, and the speaker is the latter. This is simply untrue.

In a conversation with Richard Dawkins, Neil deGrasse Tyson explains why: “If there’s any one subject that the greatest number of people say, ‘I was never good at insert a topic,’ it’s going to be math. So I say to myself, ‘If our brain were wired for logical thinking, then math would be everyone’s easiest subject, and everything else would be harder.’ I’m kind of forced to conclude that our brain is not wired for logic.”

Tyson’s right. The brain is (mostly) not hardwired for mathematics. But if that’s the case, then where did the myth of the math person come from, and how can we correct for it?

Think you’re bad at math? There’s a reason for that. (1)

While there is no innate math ability in this brain, there sure is a lot of room for math anxiety.

(Photo from Flickr)

How we know math ability isn’t genetic

The reason skill in mathematics isn’t genetically determined is because math hasn’t been around long enough to be written into our genes. As developmental psychologist Steven Pinker writes in How the Mind Works:

On evolutionary grounds it would be surprising if children were mentally equipped for school mathematics. These tools were invented recently in history and only in a few cultures, too late and too local to stamp the human genome. The mothers of these inventions were the recording and trading of farming surpluses in the first agricultural civilizations.

With that said, Pinker notes that we do come pre-equipped with some innate mathematical intuitions. For example, toddlers can choose which picture has fewer dots, children can divide snacks to share, and all cultures have words for numbers (even if that lexicon is limited to one, two, and many.) All feats managed with no formal schooling, and all evolutionary advantageous.

Citing the work of mathematician Saunders Mac Lane, Pinker speculates that these intuitions may have provided the inspiration for contemporary branches of mathematics: grouping, arithmetic, geometry, and so on.

These intuitions are not the same as the highly formal rule systems we start learning in elementary school, though. He explains the distinction as so: Anyone can tell you that cutting through a field is shorter than walking its edges, but it takes a mathematician to point out that “the hypotenuse is equal to the sum of the squares on the other two sides.”

While mathematical ability may not be congenital, it is worth noting that general intelligence is. To some degree at least. General intelligence is influenced by both genetic and environmental factors, and it can be challenging to study the complex interplay between the two. Raw intelligence will, naturally, help one acquire math skills, but as we’ll see, environmental factors should not be underplayed.

Creating a self-fulfilling prophecy

Professors Miles Kimball and Noah Smith are highly critical of the math people myth, calling it “the most self-destructive idea in America today.” Writing for the Atlantic, they argue this pernicious idea originates from a pattern children suss out when they first enter math class.

The pattern goes like this:

Some children come from homes where parents teach them math at an early age, while others are first introduced to math in school. The prepared children do well because they are already familiar with the subject matter. The unprepared children struggle because they are not.

As test and homework scores accumulate, the prepared children begin to recognized their successes. They assume they are “math people,” take pride in their achievement, learn to enjoy the subject, and push themselves to work harder.

The unprepared children, however, don’t realize that the prepared children had a head start. They assume they weren’t born “math people,” find the subject frustrating, and don’t push themselves, believing achievement will remain out of reach because of some unrecifiable deficiency.

The result is that “people’s belief that math ability can’t change becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.”

How Children Learn Language

content.jwplatform.com

Metaphorically speaking

Teachers and parents may also perpetuate the math person myth, even when trying to reduce math anxiety and encourage students that they can succeed.

Consider Dr. Randy Palisoc. He claims that math difficulties lie in our dehumanized approach toward teaching it. He believes that if we show students that math is a language “just like English, Spanish or Chinese” and that it can be used to communicate, they will recognize their natural talents and approach the subject with alacrity.

Mathematician Eddie Woo follows a similar tactic, but he relegates mathematics to a human sense, one akin to sight and touch:

Naturally some people are born with sharper sense than the rest of us; others are born with impairment. As you can see, I drew a short straw in the genetic lottery when it came to my eyesight. Without my glasses everything is a blur. I’ve wrestled with this sense my entire life, but I would never dream of saying, ‘Well, seeing has always been a struggle for me. I guess I’m just not a seeing kind of person.’

Both Ralisoc and Woo propose to reduce abstraction in the teaching of math — make it less hieroglyphics on a blackboard and more an exploration of the student’s world. That’s an admirable goal. I quote them here only to show how the metaphors teachers and parents may use to encourage unprepared students, in fact, perpetrate the genetic myth.

Woo’s argument undercuts his own point. A person born with perfect eyesight will effortlessly read the 20/20 line on an eye chart. But if you are born with poor sight, the eye chart will forever look like a lazy post-impressionist painting. Only corrective lenses, not hard work, can change this fact. He wouldn’t say, “I’m just not a seeing kind of person,” because it’s an odd thing to say. But that doesn’t make it any less true.

Similarly, math is not a language as Ralisoc claims. Language is something children master effortlessly because their brains are programmed with what linguists call “universal grammar.” Every English-speaking child knows that the sentences are spoken in Subject-Verb-Object format and that you add an s to most words to pluralize them. They manage this incredible feat without any formal schooling.The same cannot be said for their multiplication tables.

Linguist Noam Chomsky disregarded this idea: “To say that mathematics is a language is just a metaphoric use of the notion of language. […] It certainly doesn’t have the properties of human language. A human language is a natural phenomenon [while] mathematics is a human creation.”

Students know this. They understand that eyesight comes naturally, and while they may not have learned about universal grammar, they have a sense that language acquisition came easily to them. They didn’t even have to think about it.

Metaphors such as these, even if presented with encouragement, are wrong and reinforced the belief that being a math person requires being born with an innate gift for the subject.

Think you’re bad at math? There’s a reason for that. (2)

Only practice and hard work will can translate this math teacher’s blackboard for students.

(Photo from Wikimedia)

Practice makes proficient

But if math is not hardwired into us, why do some people become math people while others perpetually flounder? According to Pinker, it’s the same reason some of us play Carnegie Hall while others don’t. Practice.

“Mastery of mathematics is deeply satisfying,” Pinker writes, “but it is a reward for hard work that is not itself always pleasurable. Without the esteem for hard-won mathematical skills that is common in other cultures, the mastery is unlikely to blossom.”

To promote this sense of hard work and esteem, Kimball and Smith argue that we need to change the way we teach math and how our culture views intelligence as a whole. Namely, we need to switch from fixed-mindset mathematicians to growth-mindset ones.

Put simply, a growth mindset sees skills and intelligence as something that can be developed. Failure, in this perspective, is a learning experience that allows for a reassessment before the next attempt. A fixed mindset, on the other hand, sees skills and intelligence as something you are more-or-less born with. Failure, here, is simply evidence of one’s own inaptitude.

Kimball and Smith cite the work of psychologists Lisa Blackwell, Kali Trzesniewski, and Carol Dweck to support their argument. Dweck, et al., set up an experiment where they taught students that intelligence was “highly malleable” and could be “developed by hard work.” The experiment’s control group was only taught how memory works.

The students who learned that intelligence was malleable through hard work received higher grades, and those who switched from a fixed-mindset to a growth one showed the most improvement. The control group showed no such improvement.

Kimball and Smith also note that many East Asian countries — the ones currently dominating in math performance scores — utilize the techniques of hard work and a growth mindset as part of their culture.

Quoting an analysis by Richard Nisbett’s, they point out that children in Japan go to school 60 more days a year than U.S. students, study more hours a day, and are culturally more accustom to criticism, leading them to be more persistent to correct failures.

Think you’re bad at math? There’s a reason for that. (3)

Subscribe for counterintuitive, surprising, and impactful stories delivered to your inbox every Thursday

“We see our country moving away from a culture of hard work toward a culture of belief in genetic determinism,” Kimball and Smith conclude. “In the debate between ‘nature vs. nurture,’ a critical third element — personal perseverance and effort — seems to have been sidelined. We want to bring it back, and we think that math is the best place to start.”

True, practice and a growth mindset won’t guarantee a teaching position in Harvard’s math department. If that’s your goal, you’ll need a healthy dose of raw intelligence and luck. But Kimball and Smith’s point isn’t that we can all become math geniuses.

Instead, by replacing the math person myth with an ethos of hard work and a growth mindset, we can teach children to achieve their personal best. For most students, this will mean reaching at least high school-level proficiency, but even if it doesn’t, it will help them see failure as a chance to improve, not a source of debilitating math anxiety.

Maybe we can’t all be math people, but we can all learn to love and appreciate the Queen of the Sciences in our lives.

Tags

brainchildrenfearintelligencemathmindmotivationteaching

In this article

brainchildrenfearintelligencemathmindmotivationteaching

Think you’re bad at math? There’s a reason for that. (2024)

FAQs

What causes people to be bad at math? ›

Much like dyslexia disrupts areas of the brain related to reading, dyscalculia affects brain areas that handle math- and number-related skills and understanding. Symptoms of this condition usually appear in childhood, but adults may have dyscalculia without knowing it.

Do I have dyscalculia or am I just bad at math? ›

The most characteristic trait is experiencing difficulties when dealing with numbers, including counting and doing arithmetic. Other early signs of dyscalculia are a reliance on counting with fingers when peers have ceased the practice (this is due to difficulty learning math facts) and trouble estimating numbers.

Why am I weak at maths? ›

The primary cause of math difficulties is an inability to create a gestalt image for the concepts underlying math processes. Individuals often attempt to memorize facts instead of being able to think, reason, and problem solve with numbers. Accept Advertisem*nt cookies to view the content.

Why am I so bad at math but good at English? ›

Your proficiency in English over math might not just be about your teachers; it could also be the language itself. English's diverse numerical vocabulary and structure could be making math harder for you, compared to languages with more straightforward numerical systems.

Why are some smart people bad at math? ›

There are many reasons for a bright student to be bad at math, including poor learning environments, attention disorders and anxiety.

What causes poor math skills? ›

A student with developmental dyscalculia may have other learning differences like a reading disability or ADHD. Poor math performance may be due to outside factors such as absenteeism, changing schools, a language barrier, or something going on at home.

Why am I so bad at math but good at everything else? ›

Dyscalculia. Some people – around 7% of us – find maths difficult because of a developmental disorder called dyscalculia. Dyscalculia is specifically a mathematical learning disability: you might be very intelligent and have access to good all-round teaching, but still struggle to learn maths.

What can be mistaken for dyscalculia? ›

Dyscalculia and math anxiety often overlap. Both can affect how kids perform in math. But while these challenges sometimes look the same, they're actually different.

What are 10 symptoms of dyscalculia? ›

What to look for
  • Have difficulty recognizing numbers.
  • Be delayed in learning to count.
  • Struggle to connect numerical symbols (5) with their corresponding words (five)
  • Have difficulty recognizing patterns and placing things in order.
  • Lose track when counting.
  • Need to use visual aids — like fingers — to help count.
Mar 8, 2024

Why am I struggling so hard with math? ›

One of the most common reasons people struggle with math is that math involves abstract concepts that can be pretty difficult to understand. Unlike other subjects that are more concrete, math deals with numbers, symbols, and equations that can be difficult to grasp.

How to test for dyscalculia? ›

How it works: The child is given written tests of math computation problems. (These are similar to the problems in the computation test, but easier.) The child must complete as many problems as possible within a certain time frame. The amount of time varies by age, but it's generally between three and five minutes.

Why are some people naturally bad at math? ›

Okay, so we fibbed a little bit—some people really are naturally bad a math. Those people have a learning disability called dyscalculia. People with this disability have extreme difficulty learning basic math concepts, no matter what their ideal learning style is.

Can you be autistic and be bad at math? ›

How autism can affect math problem solving. Autistic children can experience deficits in executive functioning. This can lead to difficulties in math word problem solving as it involves: Organizing information and operations.

What is it called when you are really bad at math? ›

Dyscalculia is a learning disability in math. People with dyscalculia have trouble with math at many levels. They often struggle with key concepts like bigger vs. smaller. And they can have a hard time doing basic math problems and more abstract math.

Why do most people fail math? ›

Many students have no idea how to study, nor do they have the discipline required to succeed in mathematics. If your idea of studying for math is to stay up all night, cram for your test, or review your notes as quickly as possible in class, you're making your life a lot more complicated than it needs to be.

Is being good at math genetic? ›

BOSTON – Our ability to do math may lie in our genes. Looking at more than a thousand students in Chinese elementary schools, researchers identified genetic variants that were strongly linked to categories of mathematical and reasoning abilities.

Can you be logical but bad at math? ›

Yes. This is entirely possible and probably very common, try visiting your philosophy department to find examples. Logic itself is very different from math in the sense that mathematics, even pure set theory, requires you to know semantic definitions and understand what they mean when making your logical implications.

Why is my son so bad at math? ›

Math learning differences

Some kids struggle with math because of a learning difference called dyscalculia. Dyscalculia isn't as well-known as other learning and thinking differences, like dyslexia. But experts believe it's just as common. There are lots of tools and strategies to help kids with dyscalculia thrive.

References

Top Articles
The latest death notices across Wales as families pay tribute
Iraqi Dinar Guru Updates, Investment, News, Opinions, and Intel (08-12-2024)
How Much Does Dr Pol Charge To Deliver A Calf
Archived Obituaries
Is Csl Plasma Open On 4Th Of July
Samsung 9C8
Best Cheap Action Camera
Slapstick Sound Effect Crossword
Steve Strange - From Punk To New Romantic
MADRID BALANZA, MªJ., y VIZCAÍNO SÁNCHEZ, J., 2008, "Collares de época bizantina procedentes de la necrópolis oriental de Carthago Spartaria", Verdolay, nº10, p.173-196.
Our History | Lilly Grove Missionary Baptist Church - Houston, TX
Comenity Credit Card Guide 2024: Things To Know And Alternatives
Red Heeler Dog Breed Info, Pictures, Facts, Puppy Price & FAQs
The Weather Channel Facebook
Https //Advanceautoparts.4Myrebate.com
Everything You Need to Know About Holly by Stephen King
2024 Non-Homestead Millage - Clarkston Community Schools
978-0137606801
Otterbrook Goldens
Water Days For Modesto Ca
Bridge.trihealth
Keurig Refillable Pods Walmart
Craigslist Pinellas County Rentals
[Cheryll Glotfelty, Harold Fromm] The Ecocriticism(z-lib.org)
Nearest Walgreens Or Cvs Near Me
Noaa Duluth Mn
Mega Personal St Louis
Brbl Barber Shop
Atlases, Cartography, Asia (Collection Dr. Dupuis), Arch…
How Taraswrld Leaks Exposed the Dark Side of TikTok Fame
Is Holly Warlick Married To Susan Patton
Relaxed Sneak Animations
Sensual Massage Grand Rapids
Downtown Dispensary Promo Code
Umn Biology
Purdue Timeforge
What Happened To Father Anthony Mary Ewtn
Panchitos Harlingen Tx
Naya Padkar Newspaper Today
Go Upstate Mugshots Gaffney Sc
Tirage Rapid Georgia
About :: Town Of Saugerties
„Wir sind gut positioniert“
Telugu Moviez Wap Org
Miracle Shoes Ff6
Portal Pacjenta LUX MED
Dolce Luna Italian Restaurant & Pizzeria
Solving Quadratics All Methods Worksheet Answers
Razor Edge Gotti Pitbull Price
Bob Wright Yukon Accident
Stone Eater Bike Park
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Otha Schamberger

Last Updated:

Views: 5295

Rating: 4.4 / 5 (75 voted)

Reviews: 82% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Otha Schamberger

Birthday: 1999-08-15

Address: Suite 490 606 Hammes Ferry, Carterhaven, IL 62290

Phone: +8557035444877

Job: Forward IT Agent

Hobby: Fishing, Flying, Jewelry making, Digital arts, Sand art, Parkour, tabletop games

Introduction: My name is Otha Schamberger, I am a vast, good, healthy, cheerful, energetic, gorgeous, magnificent person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.